Beyond the Surface

Evaluating Perception and Accuracy in 3D Printed Data Visualizations

Tyler Wiederich

September 18, 2025

Overview

  • Motivation and Background
  • Projects
    1. 3D Bar Charts
    2. Experiential Learning
    3. 3D Heat Maps
  • Roadmap
  • Contribution

Motivation

Motivation

Modern statistical graphics exist largely within the confines of 2D projections.

  • Research articles
  • Textbooks
  • Documentation / reports
  • News articles

Why?

  • Easy to make / understand
  • Low cost to produce
  • Many available software options

Source: www.1011now.com

Source: www.klkntv.com

Source: Black Hills Energy

Motivation

The rise in computer graphics also included the pioneering of 3D graphical renderings 1 2. Many software programs include options for creating such charts, including:

  • R
  • Python
  • SAS
  • MATLAB
  • SPSS
  • etc.
Figure 1: 3D scatterplot of the Palmer Penguins dataset from Horst, Hill, and Gorman (2020)
(a) Microsoft® Excel 95
(b) Microsoft ® Excel for Mac, Version 16
Figure 2: Two 3D charts created with Microsoft® Excel

Motivation

To date, there is no widespread usage of 3D statistical graphics outside of 2D projections. They are generally considered to be items of interest, without a formal role in data interpretations.

Figure 3: 3D visualization of ___ in DSCI 210 at Winona State University, 2019.

Background

Background

Types of 3D charts can be grouped into one of two categories.

Decorative 3D

  • 2D charts converted drawn to have artificial axis
  • Considered to be “chart junk”1
  • Static renderings

Informational 3D

  • All axes convey information
  • Intuitive use cases
  • Static or interactive renderings

Evaluation of 3D Charts

There are many possible metrics to evaluate the qualities of a chart 1 2 3 4 5.

  • Accuracy

  • Response times

  • Pattern recognition

  • Memorability

  • Preference

Decorative 3D

The relationship between 2D and 3D bar charts presented on paper or computer screens has been widely studied.

Informational 3D

Unlike decorative 3D elements, direct comparisons of dimensionality is more limited when the third dimension coveys information.

  • 3D heat maps have lower error rates than 2D heat maps in virtual reality (Kraus et al. 2020).

Data beyond a screen

All of reported results have one key limitation: they are limited to 3D charts presented on 2D surfaces. With modern technology, we are able to create 3D charts in our 3D world via 3D-printing. The rising popularity and decreasing cost of 3D printers makes this option more feasible to implement in the communication of data.

Figure 4: 3D-printed representation of 3D printer sales. Source: Wall Street Journal (link to print)

Research Objectives

Data physicalization of statistical graphics has not been widely studied. In our research, we are broadly studying the following questions:

  • Do 3D-printed charts follow patterns found in previous studies that compared different types of charts?
  • Are 3D-printed charts better or worse than their digitized counterparts (2D and 3D renderings) regarding numerical accuracies of stimuli ratio comparisons?
  • How do students participating in our experiments reflect on the scientific process, both as a participant and when viewing the study through the eyes of a researcher?

Project 1: 3D Bar Charts

3D Bar Charts

(a) Figure from 1978 Handbook of Agricultural Charts. Enlargements / [u.s. Department of Agriculture] (1978)
(b) Figure from 1978 Handbook of Agricultural Charts. Enlargements / [u.s. Department of Agriculture] (1978)
(c) Figure from Modley (1952)
Figure 5: Three examples of 3D bar charts found in publications.

Overview of Project 1

While early testing of statistical graphics started in the early 20th century 1 2, a major study was conducted by Cleveland and McGill (1984) to provide guidance on better visualization practices. In their study, numerical estimations of stimuli ratios showed differences in various methods of data representation.

In our study, we partially replicate Cleveland and McGill’s study, including the additional factor of chart medium.

Figure 6: Examples of EPTs listed in Figure 1 of Cleveland and McGill (1984).

Cleveland and McGill (1984)

Stimuli

  • Target stimuli followed \(s_i=10 \times 10^{(i-1)/12},\quad i=1,\dots, 10\)
  • Ratios of target stimuli: 0.178, 0.261, 0.383, 0.464 (twice), 0.562, 0.681 (twice), 0.825 (twice)
(a) Stimuli Values
(b) Ratios
Figure 7: Stimuli and ratios used by Cleveland and McGill (1984).

Cleveland and McGill (1984)

Chart Types

Figure 8: Chart types used by Cleveland and McGill (1984)

  • Type 1: Position along a common scale
  • Type 2: Position along a common scale
  • Type 3: Position along a common scale
  • Type 4: Length
  • Type 5: Length

Cleveland and McGill (1984)

Experimental Design

  • Treatments
    • Ratio (10)
    • Graph type (5)
  • Responses
    • Correctly identify smaller value of stimuli pair
    • \(\log_2(|\text{Error}|+1/8)\)
  • Procedure
    • 5 practice graphs followed by 50 graphs in an identical randomized order for each participant

Project 1

Our first project partially replicates and expands on the first experiment by Cleveland and McGill (1984).

Component Our Research Cleveland and McGill
# of ratios 7 ratios 10 ratios (7 unique)
# of chart types 2 5
# of media types 3 (2D and 3D digitized, 3D printed) 1 (2D on paper)
# of charts/participant 15 50

Project 1 Chart Examples

Figure 9: Charts used in bar chart pilot study.

Project 1 Results

Figure 10: Results from pilot study conducted for the 3D bar chart experiment.

Project 3: 3D Heat Maps

3D Heat Maps

(a) Figure from for Educational Statistics et al. (1977)
(b) Figure from 3D Population Density of the US - HomeArea.com” (n.d.)
Figure 11: Examples of 3D heat maps.

Overview of Project 3

We maintain the same objective when adding information to the third dimension: does numerical accuracy of ratio estimations differ between dimensionality and projections of chart types. However, it is important to note that direct translations of 2D and 3D heatmaps require different visual cues.

(a) Flatter height scale
(b) Stretched height scale
Figure 12: Two figures representing the volcano dataset (R Core Team 2024) using different height scaling factors. Both figures use the same color scale, but there is no “correct” translation of color into the measurable height.

Stimuli Construction

The design of the 3D heat map experiment uses the method of constant stimuli: ratios are estimated with respect to one stimuli height that remains the same.

Setting 50 as the constant and 90 as the maximum, a sequence of stimuli are chosen by equally partitioning the ratios between \(50/50=1\) and \(50/90\approx0.556\). The same ratios are used when setting 50 as the maximum in the stimuli pair.

(a) Stimuli values
(b) Ratios of stimuli pairs
Figure 13: Stimuli used in 3D heat map experiment. All values are paired with the stimuli magnitude of 50.

Media Types

(a) 2D heatmap
(b) 3D Digital heatmap
(c) 3D-Printed heatmap
Figure 14: Media types using dataset 1 in the 3D heatmap experiment.

Procedure

Treatment Design

  • 9 stimuli pairs
  • 3 media types
  • 2 heat map datasets

Experiment Design

  1. Create incomplete blocks on stimuli pairs (choose 4 from 9)
  2. Randomly assign participant to block on experiment initialization
  3. Present media type x dataset in randomized order (whole-plot)
  4. Present stimuli pair in randomized order (split-plot)

Project 2: Experiential Learning

Experiential Learning

In a dual purpose role, a large sample was obtained for the two previous projects by incorporating the experiments as an experiential learning opportunity for Stat 218 students. This is a six stage project that follows students from participants to consumers of scientific knowledge.

Informed Consent

  • Obtain consent
  • Confirm age of majority (≥19)

Pre-experiment

  • How does the scientific process differ between researchers and the general population

Experiment

  • Provide completion code to show proof of experiment participation

Experiential Learning

Post Experiment

  • What was the purpose of experiment?
  • What were the hypotheses?
  • What are the sources of error?
  • What were the variables?
  • What elements of experimental design were present?

Abstract

  • What components of the experiment are now clearer?

Presentation

  • How did the information you gained from each stage differ?
  • What was emphasized in the presentation that wasn’t emphasized in the the abstract?
  • What critiques do you have of the study?
  • Did you prefer the abstract or presentation?

Roadmap

Roadmap

3D Bar Charts

  • ✅ Lit Review
  • ✅ Design
  • ✅ Data Collection
  • ✅ Analysis
  • ✅ Write-up

Experiential Learning

  • ☑️ Lit Review
  • ✅ Design
  • ➡️ Data Collection
    • ✅ 3D Bar Charts
    • ➡️ 3D Heat Maps
  • Analysis
  • Write-up

3D Heat Maps

  • ✅ Lit Review
  • ✅ Design
  • ➡️ Data Collection
  • Analysis
  • Write-up

Contribution to literature

Contribution to literature

Nearly all studies involving statistical graphics use paper-printed or digitized charts, resulting in a knowledge gap in statistical graphics presented in tangible formats. Our contributions are as follows:

  1. Explore the relationship of physical dimensionality on bar charts / heat maps using numerical accuracy of ratio estimations.
  2. Provide recommendations on the construction of 3D-printed statistical graphics.
  3. Explore how students interact with the process of scientific investigation using a “hands-on” statistical study.

Questions?

References

1978 Handbook of Agricultural Charts. Enlargements / [u.s. Department of Agriculture]. 1978. District of Columbia: United States. Department of Agriculture; U.S. Dept. of Agriculture ; Distributed by Science; Education Administration.
3D Population Density of the US - HomeArea.com.” n.d. https://www.homearea.com/featured/3d-population-density/#3128000. Accessed September 17, 2025.
Armstrong, Helen. 2016. Digital Design Theory : Readings from the Field. Design Briefs. New York, New York: Princeton Architectural Press.
Barfield, Woodrow, and Robert Robless. 1989. “The Effects of Two- or Three-Dimensional Graphics on the Problem-Solving Performance of Experienced and Novice Decision Makers.” Behaviour & Information Technology 8 (5): 369–85. https://doi.org/10.1080/01449298908914567.
Buja, Andreas, Dianne Cook, Heike Hofmann, Michael Lawrence, Eun-Kyung Lee, Deborah F. Swayne, and Hadley Wlckham. 2009. “Statistical Inference for Exploratory Data Analysis and Model Diagnostics.” Philosophical Transactions: Mathematical, Physical and Engineering Sciences 367 (1906): 4361–83. https://www.jstor.org/stable/40485732.
Cleveland, William S., and Robert McGill. 1984. “Graphical Perception: Theory, Experimentation, and Application to the Development of Graphical Methods.” Journal of the American Statistical Association 79 (387): 531–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1984.10478080.
Croxton, Frederick E., and Harold Stein. 1932. “Graphic Comparisons by Bars, Squares, Circles, and Cubes.” Journal of the American Statistical Association 27 (177): 54–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/01621459.1932.10503227.
Fischer, Martin H. 2000. “Do Irrelevant Depth Cues Affect the Comprehension of Bar Graphs?” Applied Cognitive Psychology 14 (2): 151–62. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(200003/04)14:2<151::AID-ACP629>3.0.CO;2-Z.
Fisher, Samuel H., John V. Dempsey, and Robert T. Marousky. 1997. “Data Visualization: Preference and Use of Two-Dimensional and Three-Dimensional Graphs.” Social Science Computer Review 15 (3): 256–63. https://doi.org/10.1177/089443939701500303.
for Educational Statistics, National Center, National Center for Education Statistics, United States. Office of Educational Research, Improvement. Center for Education Statistics, and Institute of Education Sciences (U.S.). 1977. The Condition of Education. 1975-1979: DHEW Publication. U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement, National Center for Education Statistics.
Horst, Allison Marie, Alison Presmanes Hill, and Kristen B Gorman. 2020. Palmerpenguins: Palmer Archipelago (Antarctica) Penguin Data. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.3960218.
Kraus, Matthias, Katrin Angerbauer, Juri Buchmüller, Daniel Schweitzer, Daniel A. Keim, Michael Sedlmair, and Johannes Fuchs. 2020. “CHI ’20: CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems.” In, 1–14. Honolulu HI USA: ACM. https://doi.org/10.1145/3313831.3376675.
Levy, Ellen, Jeff Zacks, Barbara Tversky, and Diane Schiano. 1996. “The SIGCHI Conference.” In, 42–49. Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada: ACM Press. https://doi.org/10.1145/238386.238400.
Melody Carswell, C., Sylvia Frankenberger, and Donald Bernhard. 1991. “Graphing in Depth: Perspectives on the Use of Three-Dimensional Graphs to Represent Lower-Dimensional Data.” Behaviour & Information Technology 10 (6): 459–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/01449299108924304.
Modley, Rudolf. 1952. Pictographs and Graphs : How to Make and Use Them. New York: Harper.
Murdoch, Duncan, and Daniel Adler. 2024. Rgl: 3D Visualization Using OpenGL. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rgl.
Peña, Alyssa, Eric Ragan, and Lane Harrison. 2020. “Memorability of Enhanced Informational Graphics.” Interdisciplinary Journal of Signage and Wayfinding 4 (1). https://doi.org/10.15763/issn.2470-9670.2020.v4.i1.a54.
R Core Team. 2024. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.
Siegrist, Michael. 1996. “The Use or Misuse of Three-Dimensional Graphs to Represent Lower-Dimensional Data.” Behaviour & Information Technology 15 (2): 96–100. https://doi.org/10.1080/014492996120300.
Stewart, Brandie M., Jessica M. Cipolla, and Lisa A. Best. 2009. “Extraneous Information and Graph Comprehension: Implications for Effective Design Choices.” Edited by M. Henderson. Campus-Wide Information Systems 26 (3): 191–200. https://doi.org/10.1108/10650740910967375.
Tufte, Edward R. 2001. The Visual Display of Quantitative Information. 2nd ed. Cheshire, Conn: Graphics Press.
Vanderplas, Susan, Dianne Cook, and Heike Hofmann. 2020. “Testing Statistical Charts: What Makes a Good Graph?” Annual Review of Statistics and Its Application 7 (1): 61–88. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-statistics-031219-041252.
VanderPlas, Susan, and Heike Hofmann. 2017. “Clusters Beat Trend!? Testing Feature Hierarchy in Statistical Graphics.” Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics 26 (2): 231–42. https://www.jstor.org/stable/44861949.
Zacks, Jeff, Ellen Levy, Barbara Tversky, and Diane J. Schiano. 1998. “Reading Bar Graphs: Effects of Extraneous Depth Cues and Graphical Context.” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 4 (2): 119–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.4.2.119.